How big is home field advantage in baseball?

In messages with new acquaintance jasoncards  in the wake of the most recent SABR Chicago Chapter meeting, he mentioned something about the role of psychology in home field advantage, and then I recalled an attempt I made last year to quantify home field advantage. At the time I believe my true aim was removing the effects of home field advantage from the boost that Yankee Stadium gives to Yankees’ home run totals. I had briefly thought about posting about it in and of itself, but as with many of my post ideas, just couldn’t manage the time, and soon forgot about it. Jasoncard’s comments reminded me, so I fired up my old spreadsheet, did some more work with it, and came up with the results I’m sharing with you now.

Approach

I could have attempted to do a park-by-park assessment of home field advantage, or a team-by-team assessment of the same, but these would prove difficult. It would be difficult to untangle the effects of the team’s differing abilities from the parks’ home field advantage, or the different park effects from the team’s home field advantage. But looking at home and away splits for the league as a whole gives a balanced view. A single team’s skill level does not skew the results because they play as many games as the home team as they do the away team. And a single park’s effects don’t skew the results because all parks contain an equal number of home and away games represented when using whole-season, all-teams results.

I used Fangraphs’ leaderboard for team stats, selecting both Home and Away splits in turn. This Fangraphs page lets you select a range of years over which to provide cumulative statistics. I found out that the Home and Away splits data there only seem to go back to 2002, which gave me a 17-year span to work with (2002 through 2018). To get a sense of any change or progression in home field advantage, I grouped the data into three portions, of 6 years (2002 through 2007), 6 years (2008 through 2013), and 5 years (2014 through 2018). I wanted to use multiple years at a time to limit the effects of small sample sizes.

I divided all cumulative statistics by plate appearances to turn them into rate statistics, to create a good basis for comparison. First I’ll present these rates, followed by the percent differences between them.

Home and Away rate statistics

The hitting stats:

Home and away hitting stats
2002-2007 2008-2013 2014-2018
Away Home Away Home Away Home
PA 573,544 552,045 567,637 546,923 470,077 452,489
AB 512,295 488,837 508,995 485,948 423,243 404,424
1B/PA .1546 .1575 .1525 .1551 .1472 .1503
2B/PA .0474 .0479 .0454 .0464 .0441 .0454
3B/PA .0045 .0053 .0042 .0053 .0043 .0051
HR/PA .0270 .0282 .0249 .0266 .0281 .0291
BB/PA .0818 .0880 .0803 .0869 .0781 .0840
IBB/PA .0068 .0076 .0059 .0067 .0049 .0054
HBP/PA .0094 .0098 .0082 .0086 .0093 .0094
SO/PA .1726 .1622 .1917 .1821 .2158 .2071
AVG .2614 .2699 .2532 .2626 .2485 .2573
OBP .3275 .3399 .3181 .3317 .3129 .3252
SLG .4153 .4317 .3966 .4165 .4007 .4172
OPS .7428 .7717 .7147 .7482 .7136 .7424

The baserunning stats:

Home and away base stealing stats
2002-2007 2008-2013 2014-2018
Away Home Away Home Away Home
SB/PA .0143 .0145 .0159 .0163 .0135 .0143
CS/PA .0061 .0058 .0062 .0058 .0056 .0052

The team collaborative stats:

Home and away collaborative offensive stats
2002-2007 2008-2013 2014-2018
Away Home Away Home Away Home
R/PA .1185 .1268 .1109 .1196 .1112 .1195
RBI/PA .1129 .1209 .1056 .1140 .1060 .1138
SF/PA .0071 .0076 .0067 .0072 .0064 .0069
SH/PA .0084 .0090 .0079 .0087 .0057 .0058

Analysis misgivings

Now let me state before going any further that I have misgivings about what I’m about to do. Taking percentage increases in rate numbers that have an upper bound can be a misleading practice. For example, let’s say in some fictional league, players reached base between 98% and 99% of the time. In such a league, a 1% increase in on base rate (say, from .9800 to .9898) is a tremendous increase. And the highest possible increase is about 2%. But for the leagues we know, with a range of about .280 to .400, a 1% increase represents a change from, say, .300 to .303, which is pretty much statistically insignificant. There are better ways to handle it, but it’s not something I can cover in a quick aside here. So note that all the statistics we’re talking about here are taking values well under half their possible maximums (the maximum is 1.000 for most of these, 4.000 for slugging percentage and 5.000 for OPS). When that’s the case, talking about percentage increases in rate numbers is good enough, and it makes intuitive sense to people.

So to be clear, an increase from .200 to .400 would be a doubling, so would be a 100% increase (not 20%).

Percentage increase in Home stats over Away stats

Here are the percentage differences in these numbers, from the Away numbers to the Home numbers:

Hitting stats
2002-2007 2008-2013 2014-2018
H/PA 2.4% ± 0.5% 2.8% ± 0.5% 2.8% ± 0.6%
1B/PA 1.9% ± 0.6% 1.7% ± 0.6% 2.1% ± 0.7%
2B/PA 1.2% ± 1.2% 2.2% ± 1.3% 2.8% ± 1.4%
3B/PA 19.0% ± 4.4% 25.2% ± 4.6% 18.0% ± 4.9%
HR/PA 4.4% ± 1.6% 6.8% ± 1.7% 3.7% ± 1.8%
BB/PA 7.6% ± 0.9% 8.2% ± 0.9% 7.5% ± 1.1%
IBB/PA 11.8% ± 3.4% 14.9% ± 3.8% 9.9% ± 4.4%
SO/PA -6.0% ± 0.6% -5.0% ± 0.5% -4.0% ± 0.6%
HBP/PA 4.4% ± 2.8% 5.0% ± 3.0% 1.1% ± 3.1%
AVG 3.3% ± 0.5% 3.7% ± 0.5% 3.5% ± 0.6%
OBP 3.8% ± 0.4% 4.3% ± 0.4% 3.9% ± 0.4%
SLG 4.0% ± 0.7% 5.0% ± 0.7% 4.1% ± 0.7%
OPS 3.9% ± 0.4% 4.7% ± 0.4% 4.0% ± 0.5%
Base stealing stats
2002-2007 2008-2013 2014-2018
SB/PA 1.4% ± >2.3% * 2.1% ± >2.1% * 5.6% ± >2.6% *
CS/PA -5.0% ± >3.4% * -5.9% ± >3.4% * -5.8% ± >3.9% *
Team offensive stats
2002-2007 2008-2013 2014-2018
R/PA 7.0% ± 0.8% ** 7.8% ± 0.8% ** 7.5% ± 0.9% **
RBI/PA 7.1% ± 0.8% ** 7.9% ± 0.8% ** 7.3% ± 0.9% **
SF/PA 7.7% ± >3.3% * 6.5% ± >3.4% * 8.3% ± >3.9% *
SH/PA 6.3% ± >3.0% * 9.8% ± >3.2% * 1.3% ± >3.9% *

About those error estimates

I calculated the ± error bars using the formula for the standard deviation of a binomial distribution, with Plate Appearances representing the number of trials, and the actual rate of occurrence as the probability of occurrence.

The error bar sizes represent two of these standard deviations, representing a 95% confidence interval. However, this approach isn’t quite right for the statistics marked with an asterisk (*), because PA doesn’t properly represent the number of trials. Because the actual number of trials ought to be smaller, I’ve placed a greater-than symbol in front of these symbols to demonstrate that the PA-based error bars are too small.
It also isn’t right for those statistics marked with a double asterisk (**), because a “successful trial” can add more than 1 to the statistic’s total, and because the probability of a successful trial varies greatly given the men on base and the number of outs. I’ve provided the PA-based error bars for these anyway, as a reference point.

Analysis

I must say I was surprised by how all-encompassing home field advantage turned out to be.

I’m not surprised that the biggest effect was for triples. Triples usually happen when the ball is hit to very particular parts of a ballpark, and these parts are, in many cases, unique to each park. The home team players will therefore have a better idea of when they should try to stretch a double into a triple.

But that the home team would have both more stolen bases and fewer caught stealings? That’s harder to explain. Do baserunners simply run faster in their home park? Or run smarter? Is it easier for them to focus on the pitcher’s tells at home because the familiar backdrop is less distracting? Perhaps a little of all of these. Perhaps the umpires slightly favoring the home team as well, on close plays.

Maybe the baserunners run faster because the visiting team’s locker room is smaller, more cramped? Or the visitors are more tired from travel?

You can’t make that case for the hit by pitch rate, though. If anything, the better rested team should be able to dodge an errant pitch more effectively, so the home team should have fewer of these. So it’s either the umpire, or the pitcher.

Hey – maybe I’ve been focusing on the wrong thing. Maybe it’s not that the hitters and runners on the home team do better. Maybe it’s that the pitchers on the visiting team do worse. Standing in the middle of all those thousands of people who don’t like you – the pressure has to be felt by the pitcher most. And throw a major league pitcher’s very finely-tuned control off ever so slightly, and sixty feet six inches away, you get difference between a ball and a strike, or a hittable strike and an unhittable one.  Could this be the psychological effect that Jasoncards was thinking of?

But look at those error bars on SB, CS, and HBP. These are not very significant home field advantages for the stats I’ve been discussing.

They are significant, however, for walks and strikeouts. And these are some of the larger effects we’re seeing: around -5% for strikeouts, and about +7.5% for walks. So is it the hitters, the pitchers, or the umpires making the biggest difference here? I’ll guess pitchers first, then umpires second, and hitters last.

One thing I haven’t mentioned is cheating. You’d think the home team would be more likely to have devices, people, or both planted to let them pick up signs, pitch grips, what have you, or to relay information to the players. Depends on how much of that sort of thing you think goes on. Some does, but how much?

There’s one number here that is surely immune to the effects of umpire’s calls, and to cheating, and that’s intentional walks.  Neither can intentional walks be directly attributed to the skill of the pitcher or the hitter. Yet this stat has one of the largest home team boosts, between 10% and 15% over the visiting team’s rate of intentional walks! There are two causes I can think of here:
1. Because the overall offensive performance is better for the home team, they more often end up in situations that call for an intentional walk;
2. Because the home team bats last, the visiting team has clearer choices in terms of the trade offs they can make in the final inning that will allow them to win the game, including intentional walks.

In the end, the more important stats are the traditional stats like on-base percentage, slugging percentage, and OPS, and these all show about a 4% boost for the home team. But interestingly, the most important stat of all gets a bigger boost. Runs per plate appearance is 7% to 8% higher for the home team. You’d think it would be closer in line to OPS, but it’s not. The nonlinear nature of run production versus the linear nature of OPS could explain this difference.

Advertisement

How does the number of outs per inning affect run production?

Right, I hear you.  The number of outs per inning is always 3.  So why ask how the number of outs per inning affects run production?

Well there was likely a time, somewhere in the 1800’s, when the number of outs per inning wasn’t decided for sure.  What if those crafting the game of baseball had settled on 4-out innings instead of 3-out innings?

What if those changing the game of baseball today were to settle on 4-out innings instead of 3-out innings? It may not be much more drastic than some of the changes that have been proposed.

Would 4-out innings have 4/3 the number of runs scored as 3-out innings? If you think about it, you may find some reasons why the answer is no. But what do those reasons amount to, in terms of actual numbers of runs?

There is a run scoring model that provides a prediction of what they would amount to. It’s called Expected Binomial Production (EBP). It’s nothing special – it’s middle-of-the-pack in terms of run-predicting accuracy, compared to the most well known run scoring models such as Base runs, Extrapolated Runs, and Estimated Runs Produced. But there is a key difference. Those other models were constructed with the help of empirical data, and all that empirical data is from games played with three-out innings. They therefore cannot make predictions about games played with any other number of outs per inning. EBP, on the other hand, is 100% derived, using no empirical data, and thus can be adjusted for any number of outs.

EBP makes some simplifying assumptions. Baserunners never advance, except by a hit. Baserunners never make outs, either. It turns out that this run-decreasing assumption pretty much neutralizes this run-increasing assumption, leaving EBP with little overall bias when applied to major league baseball games. This may not be the case for amateur or minor league baseball.

Because of these simplifying assumptions, EBP will not reflect how more or less conservative baserunning, or increased numbers of double plays, will affect runs per inning when the number of outs per inning is changed.  It solely reflects the change in runs scored due to fewer runners being stranded because they had extra opportunities to score.

The EBP formulas take as inputs team on base percentage, the team’s “power profile” (ratios of home runs to triples to doubles, etc.), and percentages of baserunners who take an extra base on a hit.  When using modern numbers for those things, EBP predicts that in a 4-out inning, instead of increasing by a factor of 4/3 over 3-out innings, runs per inning increases by an additional 16%.  Put another way, runs scored per out is increased by 16% when you switch from 3-out innings to 4-out innings, due to some runners that would have been stranded instead scoring.

Here is a full introduction to Expected Binomial Production.  However, more relevant to this post is this discussion of how the formula changes when numbers of outs per innings are changed, and this discussion of the meaning of the three factors that comprise the basic EBP formula.